• Dutch
  • Frisian
  • Saterfrisian
  • Afrikaans
Show all
Introduction
quickinfo

Verbs (V), nouns (N), adjectives (A) and prepositions (P) constitute the four major word classes. The Syntax of Dutch is organized around these so-called lexical heads; the remaining word classes, the so-called functional heads (such as complementizers and determiners), are discussed in relation to these lexical heads. The second part of the Syntax of Dutch deals with nouns and their projections, i.e. noun phrases. The general introduction in Chapter 15 provides a survey of the most distinctive syntactic, semantic and morphological characteristics of noun phrases, as well as a semantic classification of nouns. The remainder of the study deals with the internal structure of the noun phrase and its syntactic uses.

readmore
[+]  I.  Internal organization of the noun phrase

Roughly speaking, the noun phrase can be divided into two subdomains: the lexical domain and the functional domain. The lexical domain contains the head noun and its °arguments and modifiers (if present) and determines the denotation of the noun phrase; this domain is called the NP-domain (or NP for short). The functional domain includes the NP, but may also include elements like numerals/quantifiers and determiners, which determine the quantificational and referential properties of the noun phrase as a whole; the functional domain is called the DP-domain (or DP for short), since this is usually the highest nominal projection of the noun phrase. An example is given in (1), where “Ø” stands for the (phonologically empty) indefinite counterpart of the definite plural article dethe.

1
a. [DP dedet [NumP drie [NP kritischemod vragen over de gebeurtenisarg]]]
  the three critical questions about the event
b. [DP Ødet [NumP drie [NP kritischemod vragen over de gebeurtenisarg]]]
  the three critical questions about the event

The organization of this study reflects the internal organization of the noun phrase.

[+]  A.  NP-domain (Chapter 16 through Chapter 18)

Complementation of nouns is discussed in Chapter 16; much attention will be paid to various types of deverbal nouns, because the morphological process of nominalization usually involves inheritance of the argument structure of the input verb/adjective. This is illustrated in (2) for a nominal infinitive and a deverbal noun derived by affixation with –ing.

2
a. Jan verwoestte het zandkasteel.
  Jan destroyed the sandcastle
b. het verwoesten van het zandkasteel van/door Jan
nominal infinitive
  the destroy of the sandcastle of/by Jan
c. Jans verwoesting van het zandkasteel
ing-nominalization
  Jan’s destruction of the sandcastle

The modification of nouns is discussed in Chapter 17. Section 17.1 will first discuss the distinction between restrictive and non-restrictive modification, and Sections 17.2 and 17.3 will then discuss premodification by attributively used adjectives such as mooibeautiful in (3a), and postmodification by PPs such as op de hoekat the corner in (3b). However, by far the largest part of Section 17.3 will be devoted to NPs modified by a relative clause.

3
a. het mooie huis
premodification by AP
  the beautiful house
b. het huis op de hoek
postmodification by PP
  the house at the corner
c. het huis [dat Jan wil kopen]
postmodification by a relative clause
  the house that Jan wants buy
  'the house that Jan wants to buy'

Chapter 18 concludes the discussion of the NP domain with a discussion of binominal constructions such as een emmer perena bucket [of] pears, i.e. noun phrases containing sequences of more than one noun.

[+]  B.  DP-domain (Chapter 19 through Chapter 21)

Chapter 19 begins the discussion of the functional domain of the noun phrase with a discussion of the determiners. We will show that the determiners can be divided into at least the following subcategories: articles, demonstrative pronouns, and possessive pronouns. According to some researchers, the personal pronouns can also be considered determiners, so we will include them in this chapter.

4
a. een/het boek
article
  a/the book
b. dit/dat boek
demonstrative pronoun
  this/that book
c. haar boek
possessive pronoun
  her book

After the discussion of the syntactic and semantic properties of articles in Section 19.1, Section 19.2 presents a novel categorization of pronouns based on a strict separation of syntactic and semantic criteria, and provides a detailed description of the three main categories: personal, possessive, and demonstrative pronouns. There are good reasons to consider relative pronouns as determiners as well, but for practical reasons we will not discuss them in this chapter, but in Section 17.3.2 on relative clauses. Referential, reflexive, and reciprocal pronouns can, and sometimes must, have an antecedent within their sentence. A satisfactory introduction to such pronouns should therefore pay attention to the syntactic constraints on their interpretation, as it is not the case that they can be coreferential with just any noun phrase in the sentence. This will not be done in Section 19.2 on pronouns, but in Chapter 23, which provides a detailed discussion of the so-called binding theory, which formulates the syntactic restrictions on the use of referential personal pronouns within a single sentence, including the well-known observation that referential and complex reflexive personal pronouns like hemhim and zichzelfhimself are usually in complementary distribution; this is illustrated by the examples in (5), where italics indicate coreference between the reflexive/referential pronoun and its intended antecedent, the proper noun Jan.

5
a. Ik denk dat Jan zichzelf/*hem bewondert.
  I think that Jan himself/*him admires
  'I think that Jan admires himself.'
b. Jan denkt dat ik hem/*zichzelf bewonder.
  Jan thinks that I him/himself admire
  'Jan thinks that I admire him.'

Chapter 20 continues with a discussion of cardinal and ordinal numerals such as drie/derdethree/third and quantifiers like sommigesome and alleall. This chapter also provides a discussion of so-called quantitative er, which triggers the elision of the NP-part of noun phrases containing a numeral/quantifier; this is illustrated in example (6).

6
a. Jan heeft twee boeken en Piet heeft er [DP drie [NP e]].
  Jan has two books and Piet has er three
b. Jan heeft weinig boeken maar Marie heeft er [veel [e]].
  Jan has few books but Marie has er many

Chapter 21 concludes the discussion of the internal organization of noun phrases with a discussion of the so-called predeterminers alall and heelwhole in noun phrases like al de boekenall the books and heel de wereldthe whole world.

[+]  II.  The syntactic uses of noun phrase (Chapter 22)

Chapter 22 deals with the external distribution of noun phrases in the clause, i.e. their syntactic use as argument, predicate and adverbial phrase; cf. the examples in (7).

7
a. Mijn buurman heeft zijn auto verkocht.
argument
  my neighbor has his car sold
  'My neighbor has sold his car.'
b. Marie is (een) arts.
predicate
  Marie is a physician
  'Marie is a physician.'
c. Ik ben de hele dag vrij.
adverbial
  I am the whole day free
  'I will be off the whole day.'

Section 22.1 also contains a discussion of scrambling of arguments (i.e. their variable placement in the middle field of the clause) and expletive constructions; examples are given in (8).

8
a. Jan heeft <zijn auto> waarschijnlijk <zijn auto> verkocht.
scrambling
  Jan has his car probably sold
  'Jan probably sold his car.'
b. Er is iemand ziek.
expletive construction
  there is someone ill
  'There is someone ill.'
[+]  III.  Binding Theory

Chapter 23 concludes the discussion of nouns and their projection with a discussion of referential dependencies within the sentence, specifically the fact that referential, reflexive, and reciprocal personal pronouns can, and sometimes must, have an antecedent within their sentence. We will discuss the constraints on such referential dependencies, known as binding. Sections 23.1 to 23.3 deal with the classical binding theory as formulated in Chomsky (1981), which specifically aims to explain the complementary distribution of referential and reflexive/reciprocal personal pronouns. The reflexive pronouns discussed in these sections are the complex forms with the bound morpheme –zelf, such as zichzelfhim/herself. Section 23.4 deals with the distribution of the simplex reflexive zich, which has received a lot of attention in the literature on Dutch, but which does not seem to fall within the scope of the classical binding theory.

[+]  IV.  Important reading instruction

We conclude this brief introduction by emphasizing that, as above, we will make a terminological distinction between noun phrases, NPs, and DPs throughout this study. The first term will be used when we do not need to distinguish between the NP-domain and the DP-domain. The latter two terms, on the other hand, will be used when we focus on certain aspects of the NP or DP part of the noun phrase. For a more detailed discussion of these terms, see Section 15.1.2.

References:
    report errorprintcite